SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

Referencias del artículo

URRUTIA, MABEL  y  DE VEGA, MANUEL. Lenguaje y acción: Una revisión actual a las teorías corpóreas. RLA [online]. 2012, vol.50, n.1, pp. 39-67. ISSN 0718-4883.  http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0718-48832012000100003.

    Anderson, R. C. y Ortony, A. 1975. "On Putting Applies into Bottles - A problem of polysemy", en Cognitive Psychology 7, pp. 167-180. [ Links ]

    Austin, J. 1982. Cómo hacer cosas con palabras. Barcelona: Paidós. [ Links ]

    Aziz-Zadeh, L., Wilson, S., Rizzolatti, G. y Iacoboni, M. 2006. "Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phases describing actions", en Current Biology 16 (18), pp. 1818-1823. [ Links ]

    Badre, D. y D'Esposito, M. 2009. "Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical?", en Natural Reviews Neuroscience 10, pp. 659-669. [ Links ]

    Barsalou, L. W. 1999. "Perceptual symbol systems", en Behavioral & Brain Sciences 22, pp. 577-660. [ Links ]

    Barsalou, L. W. 2005. "Abstraction as dynamic interpretation in perceptual symbol systems". En L. Gershkoff-Stowe y D. Rakison (Eds.). Building object categories. Carnegie Symposium Series. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 389-431. [ Links ]

    Barsalou, L. W. 2008. "Grounding symbolic operations in the brain's modal systems". En G. R. Semin y E. R. Smith. Embodied grounding. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 9-42. [ Links ]

    Barsalou, L. W. 2009. "Situating concepts". En P. Robbins y M. Aydede (Eds.). The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 236-263. [ Links ]

    Barsalou, L. W. y Wiemer-Hastings, K. 2005. "Situating abstract concepts". En D. Pecher y R. Zwaan (Eds.). Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 129-163. [ Links ]

    Barsalou, L. W., Niedenthal, P., Barbey, A. y Ruppert, J. 2003. "Social Embodiment". En B. H. Ross (Ed.). The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 44. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 43-92. [ Links ]

    Barsalou, L. W, Santos, A., Simmons, W. K. y Wilson, C. D. 2008. "Language and simulation in conceptual processing". En M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg y A. Graesser (Eds.). Symbols and Embodiment. Debates on meaning and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 245-283. [ Links ]

    Beilock, S. L. y Lyons, I. M. 2009. "Expertise and the mental simulation of action". En K. Markman, B. Klein y J. Suhr (Eds.). The Handbook of imagination and mental simulation. New York: Psychology Press, pp. 21-34. [ Links ]

    Belinchón, M. 1999. "Lenguaje no literal y aspectos pragmáticos de la comprensión". En M. De Vega y F. Cuetos (Eds.). Psicolinguistica del español. Madrid: Trotta, pp. 307-373. [ Links ]

    Boulenger, V, Hauk, O. y Pulvermüller, F. 2008. "Grasping ideas with the motor system: semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension", en Cerebral Cortex 19, pp. 1905-1914. [ Links ]

    Boulenger, V., Roy, A. C, Paulignan, Y., Deprez, V., Jeannerod, M. y Nazir, T. A. 2006. "Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200 msec of processing", en Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, pp. 1607-1615. [ Links ]

    Borregine, K. L. y Kaschak, M. P. 2006. "The action-sentence compatibility effect: It's all in the timing", en Cognitive Science 30, pp. 1097-1112. [ Links ]

    Buccino, G., Riggio, L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V. y Rizzolatti, G. 2005. "Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: A combined TMS and behavioral study", en Cognitive Brain Research 24 (3), pp. 355-363. [ Links ]

    Bühler, K. 1968. Teoría del lenguaje. Madrid: Revista de Occidente. [ Links ]

    Christoff, K. y Gabrieli, J. D. E. 2000. "The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchal organization within the human prefrontal cortex", en Psychobiology 28, pp.168-186. [ Links ]

    Christoff, K., Keramatian, K., Gordon, A. M., Smith, R. y Mädler, B. 2009. "Prefrontal organization of cognitive control according to levels of abstraction", en Brain Research 1286, pp. 94-105. [ Links ]

    Clark, H. H. 1996. Using language. New York: Cambridge University Press. [ Links ]

    Cramer, S. C, Weisskoff, R., Schaechter, J. D., Nelles, G., Foley, M., Finklestein, S. P., y Rosen, B. R. 2002. "Motor cortex activation is related to force of squeezing", en Human Brain Mapping 16, pp. 197-205. [ Links ]

    Denis, M. y Le Ny, J. F. 1986. "Centering on figurative features during the comprehension of sentences describing scenes", en Psychological Research 48, pp. 145-152. [ Links ]

    De Vega, M. 1984. Introducción a la psicología cognitiva. Madrid: Alianza Universidad. [ Links ]

    De Vega, M. 1995. "Backward updating of mental models during continuous reading of narratives", en Journal of 'Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 21, pp. 373-385. [ Links ]

    De Vega, M. 2005. "Lenguaje, corporeidad y cerebro", en Revista Signos 38 (58), pp. 157-176. [ Links ]

    De Vega, M. 2008. "Levels of embodied meaning from pointing to counterfactuals". En M. De Vega, A. Glenberg y A. Graesser (Eds.). Symbols and Embodiment. Debates on meaning and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 285-308. [ Links ]

    De Vega, M. y Urrutia, M. 2011. "Counterfactual sentences may activate motoric processes. An action-sentence compatibility effect study", en Journal of Cognitive Psychology 23 (8), pp. 962-973. [ Links ]

    De Vega, M., Glenberg, A. y Graesser, A. 2008. Symbols and Embodiment. Debates on meaning and cognition. New York: Oxford, University Press. [ Links ]

    De Vega, M., Moreno, V y Castillo, D. 2011. "The comprehension of action related sentences may cause interference rather than facilitation on matching actions", en Psychological Research 14. Doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0356-1. [ Links ]

    De Vega, M., Robertson, D. A., Glenberg, A. M., Kaschak, M. P. y Rinck, M. 2004. "On doing two things at once: Temporal constraints on actions in language comprehension", en Memory and Cognition 32, pp.1033-1043. [ Links ]

    De-Wit, L., Machilsen, B. y Putzeys, T. 2010. "Predictive coding and the neural response to predictable stimuli", en The Journal of Neuroscience 30, pp. 8702-8703. [ Links ]

    Fischer, M. 2005. "Action simulation for others is not constrained by one's own postures", en Neuropsychologia 43, pp. 28-34. [ Links ]

    Fischer, M. y Zwaan, R. 2008. "Embodied language: A review of the role of motor system in language comprehension", en The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (6), pp. 825-850. [ Links ]

    Fuster, J. M. 2004. "Upper processing stages of the perception-action cycle", en Trends in Cognitive Science 8, pp. 143-145. [ Links ]

    Gallagher, S. 2009. "Philosophical antecedents of situated cognition". En P. Robbins y M. Aydede (Eds.). The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 35-51. [ Links ]

    Gallesse V., Fadiga L., Fogassi L. y Rizzolatti G. 1996. "Action recognition in the premotor cortex", en Brain 119, pp. 593-609. [ Links ]

    Gámez, G., Díaz, J. M., Marrero, H., Urrutia, M., León, I. y De Vega, M. 2010. "El papel de la dimensión aproximación-evitación en la interpretación de los verbos interpersonales". En H. Marrero, E. Gámez, J. M. Díaz, I. León, M. D. Castillo y M. Urrutia (Eds.). Motivación y emoción: cuestiones actuales. La Laguna: Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de La Laguna, pp. 67-81. [ Links ]

    Gibson, J. J. 1979. The ecological approach to visual perception. Mahwah, N. J.: Erlbaum. [ Links ]

    Glenberg, A. M. 1997. "What memory is for", en Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20, pp. 1-19. [ Links ]

    Glenberg, A. M. 2008. "Toward the integration of bodily states, language, and action". En G. R. Semin y E. R., Smith (Eds.). Embodied grounding Social, cognitive, affective and neuroscientific Approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 43-69. [ Links ]

    Glenberg, A. M. y Kaschak, M. 2002. "Grounding language in action", en Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9 (3), pp. 558-565. [ Links ]

    Glenberg, A. M., Meyer, M. y Lindem, K. 1987. "Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension", en Journal of Memory and Language 26, pp. 69-83. [ Links ]

    Gomila, M. 2008. "Mending or abandoning cognitivism?". En M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg y A. Graesser. Symbols and embodiment. Debates on meaning and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 357-374. [ Links ]

    Green, A. E., Kraemer, D. J., Fugelsang J. A., Gray, J. R. y Dunbar, K. N. 2010. "Connecting long distance: Semantic distance in analogical reasoning modulates frontopolar cortex activity", en Cerebral Cortex 20, pp. 70-76. [ Links ]

    Harnard, S. 1990. "The symbol grounding problem", en Physica D. 42, pp. 335-346. [ Links ]

    Heidegger, M. 1951. El ser y el tiempo. Madrid: Fondo de Cultura Económica. [ Links ]

    Holcomb, P. J, Kounios, J., Anderson, J. E. y West, W. C. 1999. "Dual-coding, context-availability, and concreteness effects in sentence comprehension: An electrophysiological investigation", en Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25 (3), pp. 721-742. [ Links ]

    James, W. 1891. The principles of Psychology. New York: Dover Publications. [ Links ]

    Just, M. A. 2008. "What brain imaging can tell us about embodiment meaning". En M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg y A. Graesser (Eds.). Symbols and Embodiment. Debates on meaning and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 75-84. [ Links ]

    Just, M. A., Newman, S. D., Keller, T. A., McEleney, A. y Carpenter, P. A. 2004. "Imagery in sentence comprehension: an fMRI study", en Neuroimage 21, pp. 112-124. [ Links ]

    Kaschak, M. P. y Glenberg, A. M. 2000. "Constructing meaning: The role of affordances and grammatical constructions in sentence comprehension", en Journal of Memory & Language 43, pp. 508-529. [ Links ]

    Kounios, J. y Holcomb, P. J. 1994. "Concreteness effects in semantic processing: ERP evidence supporting dual-coding theory", en Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20, pp. 804-823. [ Links ]

    Mahon, B. y Caramazza, A. 2008. "A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content", en Journal of Physiology, Paris 102, pp. 59-70. [ Links ]

    Moody, C. L. y Gennari, S. P. 2010. "Effects of implied physical effort in sensory-motor and pre-frontal cortex during language comprehension", en NeuroImage 49, pp. 782-793. [ Links ]

    Paivio, A. 1971. Imagery and verbal processes. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart y Winston. [ Links ]

    Paivio, A. 1986. Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press. [ Links ]

    Pulvermüller, F. 1999. "Words in the brain's language", en Behavioral and brain sciences 22, pp. 253-336. [ Links ]

    Pulvermüller, F 2005. "Brain mechanisms linking language and action", en Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6 (7), pp. 576-82. [ Links ]

    Pulvermüller, F 2008. "Grounding language in the brain". En M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg y A. Graesser (Eds.). Symbols and Embodiment. Debates on meaning and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 85-116. [ Links ]

    Raposo, A., Moss, H., Stamatakis, E.A., Tyler, L.K., Rizzolatti, G. y Craighero, L. 2009. "Modulation of motor and premotor cortices by actions, action words and action sentences", en Neuropsychologia 47, pp. 387-396. [ Links ]

    Rizzolatti, G. y Craighero, L. 2004. "The Mirror-Neuron System", en Annual Review of Neuroscience 27, pp. 169-192. [ Links ]

    Rizzolatti, G. y Sinigaglia, G. 2006. Las neuronas espejo. Los mecanismos de la empatía emocional. Ediciones Paidós Ibérica: Barcelona. [ Links ]

    Robbins, P. y Aydede, M. 2009. "A short primer on situated cognition". En P. Robbins y M. Aydede (Eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition. New York, Cambridge University Press, pp. 3-10. [ Links ]

    Sanford, A. 2008. "Defining embodiment in understanding". En M. De Vega, A.M. Glenberg y A. Graesser (Eds.). Symbols and Embodiment. Debates on meaning and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 181-194. [ Links ]

    Shapiro, L. 2008. "Symbolism, embodied cognition, and the broader debate". En M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg y A. Graesser (Eds.). Symbols and Embodiment. Debates on meaning and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 57-74. [ Links ]

    Searle, J. 1980. Actos de habla. Madrid: Cátedra. [ Links ]

    Stanfield, R. y Zwaan, R. 2001. "The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition", en Psychological Science 12 (2), pp. 153-156. [ Links ]

    Taylor, L. y Zwaan, R. 2009. "Action in cognition: the case of language", en Language and cognition 1 (1), pp. 45-58. [ Links ]

    Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M., Gallese, V, Danna, M., Scifo, P., Fazio, F., Rizzolatti, G., Cappa, S. y Perani, D. 2005. "Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits", en Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17 (2), pp. 273-281. [ Links ]

    Tettamanti, M., Manenti, R., Della Rosa, P., Falini, A., Perani, D., Cappa, S. y Moro, A. 2008. "Negation in the brain: Modulating action representations", en NeuroImage 43, pp. 358-367. [ Links ]

    Tomasello, M. 2008. Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA; London, England: MIT Press. [ Links ]

    Tucker, M. y Ellis, R. 2004. "Action priming by briefly presented objects", en Acta Psychologica 116, pp. 185-203. [ Links ]

    Urrutia, M., Gennari, S. y De Vega, M. 2010. Counterfactuals in action. An fMRI study. Póster presentado en el Congreso Embodied Mind: Perspectives and Limitations. Holanda (Nijmegen). [ Links ]

    Varela, F., Thompson, E. y Rosch, E. 1997. De cuerpo presente. Las ciencias cognitivas y la experiencia humana. Barcelona: Editorial Gedisa. [ Links ]

    West, W. C. y Holcomb, P J. 2000. "Imaginal, semantic, and surface-level. Processing of concrete and abstract words: An electrophysiological Investigation", en Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12, pp. 1024-1037. [ Links ]

    Wilson, CD., Simmons, W. K., Martin, A. y Barsalou, L. W. 2007. Simulating properties of abstract concepts. Conference at Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Society. New York, April 2007. [ Links ]

    Willems, R. y Casasanto, D. 2011. "Flexibility in embodied language understanding", en Frontiers in Psychology 116. Doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00116. [ Links ]

    Zwaan, R. A. 2008. "Experiential traces and mental simulations in language comprehension". En M. De Vega, A. M. Glenberg y A. Graesser (Eds.). Symbols and Embodiment. Debates on meaning and cognition. New York: Oxford, University Press, pp. 165-180. [ Links ]

    Zwaan, R. A. y Kaschak, M. 2009. "Language in the brain, body, and world". En P. Robbins y M. Aydede (Eds.). The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition.New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 368-381. [ Links ]

    Zwaan, R. A. y Madden, C. 2004. "Commentary and reply. Updating situation models", en Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 30 (1), pp. 283-288. [ Links ]

    Zwaan, R. A. y Taylor, L. 2006. "Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in language comprehension", en Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 135 (1), pp. 1-11. [ Links ]

    Zwaan, R. A. y Yaxley, R. H. 2003a. "Spatial iconicity affects semantic-relatedness judgments", en Psychonomic Bulletin Review 10, pp. 954-958. [ Links ]

    Zwaan, R. A. y Yaxley, R. H. 2003b. "Hemispheric differences in semantic-relatedness judgments", en Cognition 87 (3), pp. 79-86. [ Links ]

    Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A. y Yaxley, R. H. 2002. "Language comprehenders mentally represent the shape of objects", en Psychological Science 13, pp.168-171. [ Links ]