SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

Referencias del artículo

LIMONGI TIRADO, Roberto. The periphrastic anticipatory effect: An fMRI study of the linguistic-driven anticipatory activity of posterior brain areas in causal representation. Rev. signos [online]. 2012, vol.45, n.78, pp. 60-69. ISSN 0718-0934.  http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0718-09342012000100004.

    Blakemore, S., Fonlupt, P., Pachot-Clouard, M., Darmon, C., Boyer, P. & Meltzoff, A. (2001). How the brain perceives causality: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12(17), 3741- 3746. [ Links ]

    Buracas, G. & Boynton, G. (2007).The effect of spatial attention on contrast response functions in human visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(1), 93-97. [ Links ]

    Choi, H. & Scholl, B. (2004). Effects of grouping and attention on the perception of causality. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(6), 926-942. [ Links ]

    Fonlupt, P. (2003). Perception and judgment of physical causality involve different brain structures. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 248-254. [ Links ]

    Fox, M., Corbetta, M., Snyder, A., Vincent, J. & Raichle, M. (2006). Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. PNAS, 103(26), 10046-10051. [ Links ]

    Fugelsang, J. & Dunbar, K. (2005). Brain-based mechanisms underlying complex causal thinking. Neuropsychologia, 43(8), 1204-1213. [ Links ]

    Fugelsang, J., Roser, M., Corballis, P., Gazzaniga, M. & Dunbar, K. (2005). Brain mechanisms underlying perceptual causality. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 41-47. [ Links ]

    Gabrieli, J. & Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. (2007). Attention to neglect. Neuron, 53(6), 776-777. [ Links ]

    Gomez, C., Flores, A. & Ledesma, A. (2007). Fronto-parietal networks activation during the contingent negative variation period. Brain Research Bulletin, 73(1-3), 40-47. [ Links ]

    Heeger, D. & Ress, D. (2004). Neuronal correlates of visual attention and perception. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 339-350). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [ Links ]

    Kerzel, D., Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A. & Prinz, W. (2000). Launching the effect: Representations of causal movements are influenced by what they lead to. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 53(4), 1163-1185. [ Links ]

    Liu, X., Banich, M., Jacobson, B. & Tanabe, J. (2004). Common and distinct neural substrates of attentional control in an integrated Simon and Spatial Stroop task as assessed by eventrelated fMRI. Neuroimage, 23(3), 1097-1106. [ Links ]

    Lungu, O., Liu, T., Waechter, T., Willingham, D. & Ashe, J. (2007). Strategic modulation of cognitive control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(8), 1302-1315. [ Links ]

    Miller, E. & Cohen, J. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual review of neuroscience, 24, 167-202. [ Links ]

    Pessoa, L., Kastner, S. & Ungerleider, L. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of attention: From modulation of sensory processing to top-down control. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(10), 3990-3998. [ Links ]

    Pessoa, L. & Ungerleider, L. (2004). Top-down mechanisms for working memory and attentional processes. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 919-930). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [ Links ]

    Scholl, B. & Tremoulet, P. (2000). Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(8), 299-309. [ Links ]

    Scholl, B. & Nakayama, K. (2002). Causal capture: Contextual effects on the perception of collision events. Psychological Science, 13(6), 493-498. [ Links ]

    Song, G. & Wolff, P. (2003). Linking perceptual properties to the linguistic expression of causation. In M. Achard & S. Kemmer (Eds.), Language, culture and mind (pp. 237-250). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. [ Links ]

    Sylvester, C., Shulman, G., Jack, A. & Corbetta, M. (2007). Asymmetry of anticipatory activity in visual cortex predicts the locus of attention and perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(52), 14424-14433. [ Links ]

    Talairach, J. & Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Thieme. [ Links ]

    Tolman, E. & Brunswik, E. (1935). The organism and the causal texture of the environment. Psychological Review, 42, 43-77. [ Links ]

    Ullman, M. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92(1-2), 231-270. [ Links ]

    Wolff, P. (2003). Direct causation in the linguistic coding and individuation of causal events. Cognition, 88(1), 1-48. [ Links ]

    Wolff, P. (2007). Representing causation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(1), 82- 111. [ Links ]

    Wolff, P. & Zettergren, M. (2002). A vector model of causal meaning. In W. Gray & C. Schunn (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty-forth annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 944-949). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [ Links ]

    Wolff, P. & Song, G. (2003). Models of causation and the semantics of causal verbs. Cognitive Psychology, 47(3), 276-332. [ Links ]

    Wolff, P., Klettke, B., Ventura, T. & Song, G. (2005). Expressing causation in english and other languages. In W. Ahn, R. Goldstone, B. Love, A. Markman & P. Wolff (Eds.), Categorization inside and outside the laboratory: Essays in honor of Douglas l. Medin (pp. 29-48). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [ Links ]

    Young, M., Rogers, E. & Beckmann, J. (2005). Causal impressions: Predicting when, not just whether. Memory & Cognition, 33(2), 320-331. [ Links ]

    Young, M. & Falmier, O. (2008). Launching at a distance: The effect of spatial markers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1356-1370. [ Links ]